Loading...

Is Autistic Spectrum Disorder Genetic? Exploring the Arguments

Essay 2014 7 Pages

Health Science

Excerpt

Contents

Introduction

Twin studies

Broader phenotype

Maternal Stress

Conclusion

References

Is autistic spectrum disorder genetic?

Introduction

Autistic spectrum disorder (ASD) is a group of neurodevelopmental disorders, characterised by varying degrees of social, imaginative and communicative deficits (APA, 2013). ASD includes autism, pervasive developmental disorder, Rett’s disorder and childhood disintegrative disorder (Volkmar and Wiesner, 2009).

Some argue that ASD is genetic, with de novo mutations, copy number variations and chromosomal abnormalities, influencing an ASD individual’s behaviour (Marshall et al, 2008; Levy et al, 2011; Gilberg, 1998; Pinto et al, 2010). Others argue it may be caused by the environment (Hallmayer et al, 2011) and specific experiences, such as maternal stress during postpartum and prenatal development, may be the underlying cause (Claassen et al, 2008; Hao et al, 2012).

This essay will explore these arguments, looking at twin studies, at broader phenotype and at prenatal/postpartum development, specifically in relational to maternal stress. It will conclude that ASD is not solely genetic, but can also be influenced by environmental factors.

Twin studies

Reviewing autism’s heritability can shed light on the disorder’s nature. Heritability is the extent to which an individual’s genetics influence the behavioural variations or observed traits.

Twin studies give an indication of how genetics or environmental factors could be reflected in ASD. Early clinical studies found higher concordance rates for autism in MZ than DZ twins, ranging from 36% to 91% for MZ, yet 0% for DZ twins. There was also an 82-92% concordance rate in MZ twins for related cognitive and social impairments whilst, on average, only 20% for DZ twins (Bailey et al, 1995; Folstein and Rutter, 1977; Steffenburg et al, 1989). These studies suggest a genetic component is involved.

However, the sample sizes for all three studies were extremely small; the Steffenburg study involved only 35 autistic children. Furthermore, the majority suffered from brain damage or dysfunction, meaning the actual cause of ASD could not be conclusively identified. Foldstein and Rutter (1977) also concluded that brain damage during early childhood, together with a possible genetic predisposition, may cause autism. These findings lend support to the theory that, although ASD may have a genetic component, factors such as brain damage may interact, triggering that genetic predisposition.

More recent studies have incorporated all types of autistic spectrum disorders and used larger samples. In a population based study, Lichtenstein et al (2010) studied over 10,000 Swedish twins with ASD. They found higher concordance rates for DZ twins (14%) than in previous studies. Similarly, Hallmayer et al (2011) differentiated between narrowly defined autism and broader definitions of ASD. The MZ vs. DZ concordance rate for narrowly defined autism was, on average, 59% and 24%. With broader definitions, rates increased, except for female MZ twins.

These twin studies suggest four findings. Firstly ASD is not solely genetic, but occurs in combination with environmental factors. Secondly, the choice of definition can explain (some of the) inconsistencies in the literature, and finally earlier studies have underestimated DZ twins’ concordance rates.

Early studies placed the recurrence risk for siblings at around 3% (Smalley et al, 1988; Piven et al; 1990; Bolton et al, 1994). This low percentage casts doubt on the theory of genetic involvement in autism. Those studies looked at both younger and older siblings however stoppage factors may have had an effect and skewed results. Stoppage is where families stop having children, following the birth of an autistic child. Ozonoff et al (2011) avoided stoppage factors and looked at later siblings, finding that 18.7% of younger siblings went on to develop ASD. This suggests that the risk of recurrence may have been underestimated. Constantino found that siblings affected with ASD from multiple-incidence autistic families reported significantly more, and a wider range of, autistic traits than siblings from single-incidence families. They were also more likely to have ASD as a result of genetics causes rather than non-genetic causes (Piven and Folstein et al, 1994).

Broader phenotype

It is possible that not enough importance has been placed on the risk of autistic-like traits in these families. Although not officially recognised as ASD through DSM V, studies show an increased risk for broad phenotype in relatives of ASD individuals (Bailey et al, 1998; Piven et al, 2001; Pickles et al, 2000), especially in first-degree relatives (Bolton et al, 1994; Ben-Yizhak et al, 2011). These traits, such as behavioural or social deficits, are called broader autism phenotypes (BAP), and are associated with ASD (Wheelwright et al, 2010). They have been found in non-autistic siblings.

Whilst 10% of siblings of ASD individuals went on to develop the disorder, 20% of non-autistic siblings displayed delayed language development (Constantino et al, 2010) and their cognitive and social abilities were similar to children with ASD (Valentina et al, 2014; Gamliel et al, 2007; Bolton et al, 1994). This provides strong evidence that BAP may aggregate amongst relatives, perhaps indicating a lesser variation of autism, which is inherited by relatives of ASD individuals. One explanation could be that BAP and ASD may share a similar etiology (Ronald et al, 2011; Lundström et al, 2012).

This leads to the conclusion that, firstly, current restrictive boundaries and definitions of ASD should consider these lesser variations (Volkmar et al, 2005) and secondly, rather than ASD, it may be a susceptibility to social, cognitive and language deficits that is genetic.

Maternal Stress

The prenatal and postpartum periods are particularly sensitive times for infants, and are also when abnormalities/disorders may surface. For example, prenatal stress can reduce the circulation volume of foetal blood and affect brain structure and development (Kinney et al, 2008b). Stott and Latchford (1976) found that infants, classed as displaying behavioural disturbances, were significantly more likely to have mothers who’d experienced emotional stress in prenatal/postpartum. They were also more likely to suffer neurobehavioural difficulties (Ronald et al, 2011).

Ward (1990) found that infants, whose mothers had suffered stressors in prenatal/postpartum stages, went on to develop early infantile autism. (Ward, 1990). The timing of the occurrence of the stressors was also important. Beversforf et al (2005) found a significantly higher number of stressors experienced between 21-32 weeks gestation, peaking between 25-28 weeks in ASD. This was not seen with Down's Syndrome mothers or controls. Although the effect was small, the findings were significant.

Kinney et al (2008) reviewed birth rates, ASD prevalence and the weather in Louisiana. Infants exposed to stormy weather during either their fifth, sixth or final prenatal month had increased susceptibility to ASD than those exposed to the same factors during the other months of gestation. This further supports the notion that environmental factors contribute to ASD.

Though environmental factors should be given more importance (Chaste and Leboyer, 2012; Yu et al, 2015), as with the MZ and DZ twins, results are not conclusive. Not all infants whose mothers experienced stress developed ASD. Therefore, maternal stress cannot account for ASD, by itself (Jones et al, 2010).

Conclusion

Although MZ concordance rates indicate it, heritability and genetics cannot fully explain ASD. BAP shows traits associated with relatives of ASD, although it may not meet the DSM-V criteria. This suggests that in addition to ASD, a lesser type of ASD may be caused by genetics. Recent evidence points towards the involvement of environmental factors explaining some, if not all, incidences of ASD.

To conclude, no single theory can account for ASD and it is not a solely a genetic disorder. Rather, multiple factors contribute (genetic, epigenetic, and environmental) and are involved in influencing ASD.

References

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

Bailey, A., Le Couteur, A., Gottesman, I., Bolton, P., Simonoff, E., Yuzda, E., & Rutter, M. (1995). Autism as a strongly genetic disorder: evidence from a British twin study. Psychological medicine, 25 (01), 63-77.

Bailey, A., Palferman, S., Heavey, L. and Le Couteur, A. (1998). Autism: the phenotype in relatives. Journal of autism and developmental, 28(5), p.369.

Ben-Yizhak, N., Yirmiya, N., Seidman, I., Alon, R., Lord, C., & Sigman, M. (2011). Pragmatic language and school related linguistic abilities in siblings of children with autism. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 41 (6), 750-760.

Beversdorf, D. Q., Manning, S. E., Hillier, A., Anderson, S. L., Nordgren, R. E., Walters, S. E., ... & Bauman, M. L. (2005). Timing of prenatal stressors and autism. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 35(4), 471-478

Bolton, P., Macdonald, H., Pickles, A., Rios, P., Goode, S., Crowson, M., Bailey, A. and Rutter, M. (1994). A Case-Control Family History Study of Autism. J Child Psychol & Psychiat, 35(5), pp.877-900.

Chaste, P., & Leboyer, M. (2012). Autism risk factors: genes, environment, and gene-environment interactions. Dialogues Clin Neurosci, 14(3), 281-92.

Claassen, M., Naudé, H., Pretorius, E., & Bosman, M. C. (2008). The contribution of prenatal stress to the pathogenesis of autism as a neurobiological developmental disorder: a dizygotic twin study. Early Child Development and Care, 178(5), 487-511.

Constantino, J., Zhang, Y., Frazier, T., Abbacchi, A. and Law, P. (2010). Sibling Recurrence and the Genetic Epidemiology of Autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(11), pp.1349-1356.

Folstein, S., & Rutter, M. (1977). Infantile autism: a genetic study of 21 twin pairs. Journal of Child psychology and Psychiatry, 18 (4), 297-321.

Gamliel, I., Yirmiya, N. and Sigman, M. (2007). The Development of Young Siblings of Children with Autism from 4 to 54 Months. J Autism Dev Disord, 37(1), pp.171-183.

Gillberg, C. (1998). Chromosomal disorders and autism. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 28 (5), 415-425.

Gizzonio, V., Avanzini, P., Fabbri-Destro, M., Campi, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2014). Cognitive abilities in siblings of children with autism spectrum disorders. Experimental brain research, 232 (7), 2381-2390.

Hallmayer, J. (2011). Genetic Heritability and Shared Environmental Factors Among Twin Pairs With Autism. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 68(11), p.1095.

Hao, G. J., Xue, S. A., Ki, J. C. Y., & de Schepper, L. (2012). A preliminary investigation of prenatal stress and risk factors of autism spectrum disorder. Autism Insights, 4, 15.

Jones, K. L., Smith, R. M., Edwards, K. S., Givens, B., Tilley, M. R., & Beversdorf, D. Q. (2010). Combined effect of maternal serotonin transporter genotype and prenatal stress in modulating offspring social interaction in mice. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 28 (6), 529-536.

Kinney, D. K., Miller, A. M., Crowley, D. J., & Gerber, E. (2008a). Autism prevalence following prenatal exposure to hurricanes and tropical storms in Louisiana. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 38(3), 481-488.

Kinney, D. K., Munir, K. M., Crowley, D. J., & Miller, A. M. (2008b). Prenatal stress and risk for autism. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 32(8), 1519-1532.

Lundström, S., Chang, Z., Råstam, M., Gillberg, C., Larsson, H., Anckarsäter, H., & Lichtenstein, P. (2012). Autism spectrum disorders and autisticlike traits: similar etiology in the extreme end and the normal variation. Archives of general psychiatry, 69 (1), 46-52.

Levy, D., Ronemus, M., Yamrom, B., Lee, Y. H., Leotta, A., Kendall, J., ... & Buja, A. (2011). Rare de novo and transmitted copy-number variation in autistic spectrum disorders. Neuron, 70 (5), 886-897.

Lichtenstein, P., Carlström, E., Råstam, M., Gillberg, C. and Anckarsäter, H. (2010). The Genetics of Autism Spectrum Disorders and Related Neuropsychiatric Disorders in Childhood. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(11), pp.1357-1363.

Marshall, C. R., Noor, A., Vincent, J. B., Lionel, A. C., Feuk, L., Skaug, J., ... & Thiruvahindrapduram, B. (2008). Structural variation of chromosomes in autism spectrum disorder. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 82 (2), 477-488.

Ozonoff, S., Young, G. S., Carter, A., Messinger, D., Yirmiya, N., Zwaigenbaum, L., ... & Hutman, T. (2011). Recurrence risk for autism spectrum disorders: a Baby Siblings Research Consortium study. Pediatrics, 128 (3), e488-e495.

Pickles, A., Starr, E., Kazak, S., Bolton, P., Papanikolaou, K., Bailey, A., Goodman, R. and Rutter, M. (2000). Variable Expression of the Autism Broader Phenotype: Findings from Extended Pedigrees. J Child Psychol & Psychiat, 41(4), pp.491-502.

Pinto, D., Pagnamenta, A.T., Klei, L., Anney, R., Merico, D., Regan, R., Conroy, J., Magalhaes, T.R., Correia, C., Abrahams, B.S. and Almeida, J., (2010). Functional impact of global rare copy number variation in autism spectrum disorders. Nature, 466 (7304), pp.368-372.

Piven, J. (2001). The broad autism phenotype: A complementary strategy for molecular genetic studies of autism. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 105(1), pp.34-35.

Piven, J., Gayle, J., Chase, G. A., Fink, B., Landa, R., Wzorek, M. M., & Folstein, S. E. (1990). A family history study of neuropsychiatric disorders in the adult siblings of autistic individuals. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 29 (2), 177-183.

Piven, J., Wzorek, M., Landa, R., Lainhart, J., Bolton, P., Chase, G. A., & Folstein, S. (1994). Personality characteristics of the parents of autistic individuals. Psychological medicine, 24 (03), 783-795.

Ronald, A., Pennell, C. E., & Whitehouse, A. J. (2011). Prenatal maternal stress associated with ADHD and autistic traits in early childhood. Frontiers in psychology, 1(223).

Smalley, S. L., Asarnow, R. F., & Spence, M. A. (1988). Autism and genetics: a decade of research. Archives of general Psychiatry, 45 (10), 953-961.

Steffenburg, S., Gillberg, C., Hellgren, L., Andersson, L., Gillberg, I. C., Jakobsson, G., & Bohman, M. (1989). A twin study of autism in Denmark, Finland, Iceland, Norway and Sweden. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30 (3), 405-416.

Stott, D. H., & Latchford, S. A. (1976). Prenatal antecedents of child health, development, and behavior: an epidemiological report of incidence and association. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 15(1), 161-191.

Volkmar, F. (2005). Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

Volkmar, F. R., & Klin, A. (2005). Issues in the classification of autism and related conditions. Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders, Volume 1, Third Edition, 5-41.

Volkmar, F. and Wiesner, L. (2009). A practical guide to autism. Hoboken, N.J.: John Wiley & Sons.

Ward, A. J. (1990). A comparison and analysis of the presence of family problems during pregnancy of mothers of “autistic” children and mothers of normal children. Child psychiatry and human development, 20(4), 279-288.

Wheelwright, S., Auyeung, B., Allison, C., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2010). Defining the broader, medium and narrow autism phenotype among parents using the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ). Molecular autism, 1 (1), 1.

Yu, L., Wu, Y., & Wu, B. L. (2015). Genetic architecture, epigenetic influence and environment exposure in the pathogenesis of Autism. Science China Life Sciences, 58(10), 958-967.

[...]

Details

Pages
7
Year
2014
ISBN (eBook)
9783668548480
File size
957 KB
Language
English
Catalog Number
v377438
Institution / College
Brunel University
Grade
B
Tags
genetics neurodevelopmental autistic autisticspectrumdisorder mutations chromosomal abnormalities twinstudies environmental mztwins dztwins psychology abnormal

Author

Share

Previous

Title: Is Autistic Spectrum Disorder Genetic? Exploring the Arguments